Expectancy Confirmation Processes Arising in the Social Inte

Expectancy Confirmation Processes Arising in the Social Inte

Postby admin » Thu Jun 12, 2014 8:01 am

PART 1 OF 2

EXPECTANCY CONFIRMATION PROCESSES ARISING IN THE SOCIAL INTERACTION SEQUENCE
by John M. Darley, Princeton University & Russell H. Fazio, Indiana University

NOTICE: THIS WORK MAY BE PROTECTED BY COPYRIGHT

YOU ARE REQUIRED TO READ THE COPYRIGHT NOTICE AT THIS LINK BEFORE YOU READ THE FOLLOWING WORK, THAT IS AVAILABLE SOLELY FOR PRIVATE STUDY, SCHOLARSHIP OR RESEARCH PURSUANT TO 17 U.S.C. SECTION 107 AND 108. IN THE EVENT THAT THE LIBRARY DETERMINES THAT UNLAWFUL COPYING OF THIS WORK HAS OCCURRED, THE LIBRARY HAS THE RIGHT TO BLOCK THE I.P. ADDRESS AT WHICH THE UNLAWFUL COPYING APPEARED TO HAVE OCCURRED. THANK YOU FOR RESPECTING THE RIGHTS OF COPYRIGHT OWNERS.


Image

[Commission Counsel William B. Lytton, Esq.] Do you know a police officer by the name of Delores Thompson?

[Louise James, Former MOVE member] Yes, I do.

[Commission Counsel William B. Lytton, Esq.] Do you recall telling her that your son Frank was ordered to attack you, that you were beaten until you started to vomit violently ...

Image

that Frank then placed a pillow over your face and asked John Africa if he wanted you to be cycled or killed, and that John Africa replied, “Not at this time”?

Image

[Louise James, Former MOVE member] Did she tell you that I said that?

[Commission Counsel William B. Lytton, Esq.] I have her report of that conversation, Mrs. James.

Image

[Louise James, Former MOVE member] Okay. Well, if she – were you satisfied with what she said?

[Commission Counsel William B. Lytton, Esq.] I’m asking you.

[Louise James, Former MOVE member] Okay.

Image

I heard rumors to the effect that my son had beaten me. I’ve also heard rumors to the effect that Wilson Goode put his wife’s jaw on a pulley ...

Image

that he beat the hell out of her.

Image

Will you ask Mayor Good that when he comes in here if he in fact beat his wife?

Image

And if not, why not? If it is relevant that my son beat me or whether or not he did beat me ...

Image

then I would say it is just as relevant for you to ask Wilson Goode when he comes in here, “Did he beat his wife?”

Image

I don’t think you’re gonna ask him that. I really don’t.

Image

But you should, if you feel that it is relevant.

Image

Image

Image

[Laverne Sims, Former MOVE member] Mr. Lytton, I just want to make it clear in my mind so that I understand.

Image

Am I to assume that the bomb was dropped on MOVE people ...

Image

because Frank beat his mother?

--Let the Fire Burn, directed by Jason Osder -- Illustrated Screenplay & Screencap Gallery


ABSTRACT: Psychologists have begun to use Merton's concept of the self-fulfilling prophecy to explain a wide variety of social phenomena and problems. The present article further develops the theory behind this and related concepts. The core of our argument is that self-fulfilling prophecy effects occur when any one of many possible forces distort the processes occurring in normal social interactions. To elucidate this argument, we describe a model of simple social interactions that involves (a) a perceiver's formation of an expectancy about a target person, (b) his or her behavior congruent with the expectancy, (c) the target's interpretation of this behavior, (d) the target's response, (e) the perceiver's interpretation of the response, (f) the target's interpretation of his or her own response. We discuss the biasing factors that may lead to self-fulfilling prophecy effects at each step of this sequence. In addition, we suggest several other forms of expectancy confirmation that may occur via the social interaction process. Our presentation has two major purposes: to increase the theoretical clarity of the self-fulfilling prophecy and related effects and to identify needs for future research on the topic.

The concept of the self-fulfilling prophecy continues to generate research (e.g., Crano & Mellon, 1978; Snyder & Swann,. 1978), theory (R. A. Jones, 1977), and controversy (Archibald, 1974; Wilkins, 1977). This activity is certainly a testimony to the importance of the construct, which is central to many of the practical implications of psychological knowledge for real-world settings (Rosenhan, 1973; Rosenthal & Jacobson, 1968). Partly because of this enormous practical importance, the majority of empirical investigations of the self-fulfilling prophecy have involved its occurrence in classroom settings. Rosenthal and Jacobson (1968) contended that the induction of a teacher's expectation of improvement in certain children's achievement performance caused such an improvement. Despite the controversy regarding this initial research (Elashoff & Snow, 1971; Jensen, 1969; Thorndike, 1968), subsequent experimental investigations (e.g., Meichenbaum, Bowers, & Ross, 1969; Zanna, Sheras, Cooper, & Shaw, 1975) have confirmed that teacher expectations affect student achievement.

Several recent studies, aimed at documenting the workings of the teacher-student expectancy confirmation in nonexperimental social interaction settings, have provided some estimates of the magnitude and generality of the effect. A series of correlational studies showed that naturally occurring teacher expectancies are related to students' achievement (e.g., Palardy, 1969; Rist, 1970; Seaver, 1973; Sutherland & Goldschmid, 1974). For example, Seaver (1973) suggested that teachers form expectations about a younger sibling on the basis of the older sibling's performance. He found a greater relationship between the achievement scores of two siblings taught by the same teacher than between the achievement scores of control siblings taught by different teachers. Such correlational findings are always open to question concerning causal direction. A recent cross-lagged panel analysis suggests, however, that it is the teacher expectations that affect student performance. Crano and Mellon (1978) found that teacher attitudes affected children's achievement to a greater degree than students' performance impinged on the teachers' attitudes. This is impressive testimony to the practical importance of expectancy effects.

The expectancy confirmation effect has also been investigated in contexts other than the classroom. Some of the earliest research demonstrated expectancy confirmation within the experimental situation. Under certain conditions experimenters may inadvertently bias the behavior of subjects. (See Rosenthal, 1976, for a summary of these research findings.) Furthermore, self fulfilling prophecies have bee shown to operate so as to confirm various social stereotypes. Expectancy effects have been implicated in the creation of apparent sex differences in behavior (Zanna & Pack, 1975), in the maintenance of racial stereotypes (Cooper & Fazio, 1979; Word, Zanna, & Cooper, 1974), in the perseverance of trait inferences regarding physically attractive women (Snyder, Tanke, & Berscheid, 1977), in the job performance of workers (King, 1971), and in the channeling of general social interactions (S. C. Jones & Panitch, 1971; Snyder & Swann, 1978).

Despite the practical importance of expectancy effects and despite the voluminous research literature documenting such effects, relatively little attention has been given to the process by which sell-fulfilling prophecies occur. We shall attempt to analyze this underlying social interaction process by presenting the steps in a general social interaction sequence and by examining the research documentation for each of these steps. [1]

The General Social Interaction Sequence

As recognized by researchers of widely differing orientations (e.g., signal detection theorists, Lindsay & Norman, 1977, and symbolic interaction theorists, Blumer, 1969), perception is a constructive, interpretative process. Such interpretation is particularly critical in the perception of other people. The actions of another person do not automatically convey meanings, but are given meanings by the perceiver. Therefore, a generally accepted account of a social interaction sequence between two people might be as follows: (1) Either because of past observations of the other or because of the categories into which he or she has encoded the other, a perceiver develops a set of expectancies about a target person. (2) The perceiver then acts toward the target person in a way that is in accord with his or her expectations of the target person. (3) Next, the target interprets the meaning of the perceiver's action. (4) Based on the interpretation, the target responds to the perceiver's action, and (5) the perceiver interprets the target's action. At this point, the perceiver again acts toward the target person and so can be regarded as reentering the interaction sequence loop at Step 2.

It is useful to regard the sequence as containing one more step: (6) After acting toward the perceiver, the target person interprets the meaning of his or her own action. Ordinarily, of course, the interpretation will be that the action was the appropriate one and was "caused" by the perceiver's action to which it was the response. However, other possibilities do exist. From his or her action, the individual may infer something new about himself or herself. As a result, the individual's self-concept may be modified.

This interaction sequence is arbitrary in at least two senses. First, the sequence can represent a cyclical process in that the perceiver's expectancy in Step 1 may have arisen from previous "passes" through the sequence, or it may actually mark the beginning of the interaction. Second, although we have arbitrarily designated one of the individuals involved in the interaction as the perceiver and the other as the target, it should be clear that the process can be symmetric in nature. Just as the perceiver has an impression of the target, the individual we have designated as target is likely to have expectancies concerning the perceiver. In another sense, of course, the identification of the perceiver in the self-fulfilling prophecy sequence is often nonarbitrary, and the interaction is nonsymmetric. The perceiver is the participant who generally has the power to impose his or her definition of the situation on, or affect the life course of, the other individual.

Having presented the general interaction sequence that organizes our analysis, we can now turn to the working definition of the term self-fulfilling prophecy and can examine our definition in the context of the interaction sequence. The concept of the self-fulfilling prophecy was first set forth by Robert Merton (1948, 1957):

Definitions of a situation (prophecies or predictions) become an integral part of the situation and thus affect subsequent developments .... The self-fulfilling prophecy is, in the beginning, a false definition of the situation evoking a new behavior which makes the originally false conception come true. The specious validity of the self-fulfilling prophecy perpetuates a reign of error. For the prophet will cite the actual course of events as proof that he was right from the very beginning. (Merton, 1957, p. 423)


Implicit in Merton's characterization is an image of an interaction in which one individual imposes an expectancy on a target individual in such a way as to make the second individual behave in a fashion which confirms that expectancy. Two related possibilities immediately suggest themselves: First, the behavior of the target person is not actually altered by the expectation of the perceiver and thus does not "objectively" confirm the expectation. But because of well-known perceptual distortion processes, it is interpreted by the perceiver as confirming his or her expectation. Second, the perceiver's expectation, communicated through his or her action, can actually alter the behavior of the target in such a way that observers judge the behavior to be in accordance with the definition implicit in the perceiver's expectancy. (Compare this with Rosenthal's, 1976, distinction between noninteractional and interactional experimenter expectancy effects.) The latter possibility seems most consistent with Merton's characterization of the concept, and it is the one we adopt in this article. The self-fulfilling prophecy, then, refers to the process by which the expectancies held by one individual about another alter the behaviors of that other in ways that observers would interpret as confirming the perceiver's expectancy.

Within the general interaction sequence outlined above, it is clear that Merton's definition and most research on the self-fulfilling prophecy involve the link between Step 1 and Steps 4 and 5. That is, the perceiver's expectancy evokes a new response from the target (Step 4), which fulfills the expectation. Step 5 represents the perceiver's "biased proof" that his or her expectancy regarding the target was correct. Clearly, several intermediary steps must be assumed to take place, but research on these linkages is relatively sparse. It is the purpose of this article to indicate what psychological theory might suggest about these intermediary linkages and to indicate what expectancy-related phenomena other than self-fulfilling prophecies might occur in the interaction sequence.

The Perceiver Forms An Expectancy

The first step in the interaction sequence is the perceiver's formation of an expectancy regarding the behavior of a target person. These expectancies can be about the specific behaviors predicted to be performed by the target person. However, attributional theories (Heider, 1944, 1958; E. E. Jones & Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967) suggest that the expectancies are more likely to be about the intentions and dispositions of the target person, from which general classes of behavior are predicted. Whatever the exact nature of the expectancy, it can be inferred from any of several sources. For each kind of inference, various possible sources of bias are possible.

DIRECT OBSERVATION OF BEHAVIOR

First, the perceiver necessarily observes only a sample of behaviors of the target person. It is possible that this sample of behaviors was an unrepresentative one in that the target person was acting under some constraints of which the perceiver was unaware. An unlucky undergraduate may be suffering from an attack of flu and therefore do poorly on a test on which he or she would ordinarily do well. An observer who is unaware of the illness may form an expectation of future poor performance.

Second, whether or not the sequence of behavior observed is an unrepresentative one, the inferences that the observer draws from it may be unwarranted. The actor-observer effect postulated by E. E. Jones and Nisbett (1971) bas been confirmed by other studies (see E. E. Jones, 1976, for a review of these findings). Apparently, even if an observer is aware of the constraints limiting the freedom of action of an individual, he or she is likely to underestimate the force of these constraints and still make attributions to the target person (E. E. Jones, 1979; E. E. Jones & Harris, 1967; Ross, 1977; Snyder & Jones, 1974).

The above discussion has assumed a perceiver who is simply observing the behavior of a target individual. But often the perceiver may be involved in the sequence of actions from which he or she draws inferences about the target person. The perceiver may be involved because the outcomes produced by the target are personally relevant (cf. E. E. Jones & Davis's 1965, concept of hedonic relevance) or because he or she is a participant in the sequence. Both kinds of involvement have been shown to produce different evaluations of the target person than would otherwise be made (Chaikin & Cooper, 1973; E. E. Jones & DeCharms, 1957; Miller, Norman, & Wright, 1978).

Attribution theorists have discussed the conditions under which a specific behavior of a target will be judged correspondent to the underlying disposition of the actor and will lead to the expectation of disposition-related behaviors in the future. Implicit personality theorists (see Schneider, 1973, for a review) have pointed out the possibility of a related but different process that can create expectations for new classes of behavior to be exhibited by the target person. Following ordinary attribution processes, a perceiver may infer a trait or disposition of the target person. In the perceiver's naive theory of personality, this trait will be connected to other traits, and so the perceiver expects the target to exhibit behaviors dispositionally related to those inferred traits as well. Obviously these expectations of behavior stemming from inferred traits can be different depending on the implicit personality theory of the perceiver. Furthermore, depending on how appropriate the perceiver's implicit personality theory is for the target person, these inferences may lead to inaccurate expectancies regarding the future behavior of the target.

CONTINUING SEQUENCES

In the previous examples, direct observation of a sample of a single individual's behavior led to predictions about similar or inferentially related future behaviors on the part of that individual. But not all behavioral expectancies are derived from direct observation of an individual target. Frequently, initial expectancies about the behavior of an individual are drawn from evidence about the class of individuals to which that target individual is assumed to belong (cf. E. E. Jones & McGillis's 1976, related distinction between target and category-based expectancies). A perceiver's knowledge that a target individual belongs to some racial, ethnic, or gender category may trigger inferences about what actions he or she ought to display (see Hamilton, 1979, for a review). Thus stereotypes operate in a manner similar to the operation of implicit theories of personality. Both constructs involve the interconnection of various attributes. In fact, stereotypes may be regarded, as R. A. Jones (1977) has cogently argued, as regions of an individual's implicit personality theory. Like implicit personality theories, stereotypes can vary with regard to their appropriateness and accuracy. However, a great deal of the thrust of stereotyping research has been to demonstrate that these behavioral expectancies are overgeneralized and inaccurate predictors of actual behavior of the target individual. (For a review, see Brigham, 1971.)

One difference between implicit personality theories and stereotypes is in the cue that generates the inferential process. Generalization via an implicit personality theory occurs subsequent to the attribution of a particular trait to a target person. Stereotypes, on the other hand, are generated by the observation that an individual is a member of a particular group or category.

Finally, expectations about others can be derived from third parties. For instance, people's reputations may be known before their actual arrival and may set up expectations about their behavior. In experimental studies, expectations are frequently created in this fashion; for instance, in the well-known Rosenthal and Jacobson (1968) study, expectations about specific children who would be "late bloomers" were conveyed to the teachers by the psychologist.

The Perceiver Acts

Once the opportunity for a perceiver-target interaction arises, the perceiver's expectancy regarding the target (however that expectancy was formed) influences his or her behavior toward the target. By guiding the perceiver's actions, the expectancy channels the course of the perceiver-target interaction. This channeling can occur via different types of action on the part of the perceiver. It will be useful to distinguish between actions that terminate the interaction sequence and those that permit it to continue.

TERMINATED SEQUENCES

On the basis of his or her expectancy, the perceiver may actively avoid the initiation of any interaction with the target. Consistent with Newcomb's (1947) autistic hostility hypothesis, the perceiver may ignore any overtures on the part of the target and, if socially forced into the interaction, may seek to terminate it at the earliest possible moment. For example, a bigot is unlikely to seek out interactions with a black (DeFleur & Westie, 1958; Warner & DeFleur, 1969). Such a case is most apt to occur when the perceiver's expectancy concerning the target's behavior is so negative or ego threatening that he or she wishes to avoid contact with the target. The end result of such expectancy-guided avoidance behavior is the maintenance of the perceiver's impression of the target. By refusing to begin or by quickly terminating the interaction, the perceiver never allows the target an opportunity to correct any misperceptions. The perceiver's hostility has become "autistic" in the sense that it prevents him or her from being responsive to opportunities for interaction that might serve to provide additional information about the target person.

Of course, termination of the interaction by the perceiver may strongly affect the target's life if the perceiver holds some power over the target. On the basis of some quickly formed expectancy, an advisor may decide to dismiss a student or an employer may not hire a prospective employee.

Therefore, although the fact that a perceiver's categorizations may lead to a false diagnosis of the target is obvious enough, it is possible that its role in the production of self-fulfilling prophecies is underestimated. In the real world, high-power individuals often make treatment decisions on the basis of extraordinarily little evidence because they are under great time pressure. The psychiatrist, the teacher, and the police officer often need to make rapid assessments of persons. On the basis of those assessments, they may assign the individual to a specific treatment program. The interaction between the authority figure and the target has now terminated, but the authority's influence on the life of the target may be decisive.

CONTINUING SEQUENCES

Even if the perceiver's expectations do not lead to avoidance or premature termination of the interaction, they may still affect its course. In such cases the perceiver's expectation influences the manner in which he or she behaves toward the target, thus coloring the nature of the ensuing interaction. The link between perceiver expectancy and action has been widely demonstrated. In an observational study of a kindergarten class, Rist (1970) found that the teacher divided the children into rigid tracks very early in the year. This division appeared to he largely based on socioeconomic status and appearance qualities that the teacher considered to be indicative of academic promise. Not only did the teacher physically separate the different tracks, but she spent a disproportionate amount of time teaching the highest track and disciplining the lowest track. Thus teaching behavior was related to the teacher's expectations regarding the academic ability of the children.

Many experimental studies involving classroom or tutoring situations have demonstrated differential teacher behavior as a function of expectancy. Relative to controls, students labeled as bright have been found to receive a greater percentage of teacher interactions characterized by positive affect or a lesser percentage of interactions characterized by negative affect (e.g., Meichenbaum et al., 1969), more positive nonverbal behaviors [2] (e.g., smiling, direct eye gaze) from the teacher (e.g., Chaikin, Sigler, & Derlega, 1974), higher levels of positive reinforcement following correct responses, and higher levels of negative reinforcement following incorrect responses (e.g., Lanzetta & Hannah, 1969). In a review of many such classroom studies, Rosenthal (1974) bas cited evidence that teachers provide children labeled as bright with a warmer climate, more differentiated performance feedback, a greater amount of and more difficult material to learn, and mort opportunities to respond. (See also Cooper, 1979, for a review.)

The expectancy-action link has also been demonstrated by numerous studies involving interactions other than teacher-pupil ones. Subjects have been found to behave more competitively in a game situation when led to expect that their partner disliked them (S. C. Jones & Panitch, 1971) or that their partner was generally a hostile person (Snyder & Swann, 1978). Kelley (1950) found that participation in a class discussion varied as a function of whether the students expected a guest instructor to be a warm or cold person. In the context of an interracial job interview, Word et al. (1974) found that trained white and black confederates posing as job applicants were treated differently by white job interviewers. Relative to the white applicants, black applicants received shorter amounts of interview time, greater physical distancing, and higher rates of speech error (e.g., repetitions, stutters) from the white interviewers. These findings document the effect of stereotypic assessments of a target person on behaviors toward that person.

At this point it again becomes obvious that the interaction process is symmetrical in nature. The target, in effect, becomes a perceiver. Because the target's response is influenced by the interpretation given to the perceiver's action, it is important to consider how the action is apt to he perceived. First, the target is likely to have some perceptions of the perceiver that lead to expectations about the personality and actions of the perceiver. (These expectancies arise from the processes by which the perceiver categorized the target person, which we discussed earlier, and are subjected to the same potential biases.) In addition the target person has available the actions of the perceiver and will need to interpret their meaning.

The Target Interprets the Perceiver's Action

The perceiver has perceived and acted toward the target, and now the target may interpret the meaning of the perceiver's actions. Attribution theory gives a cognitive, decision-making account of a person's interpretations of the acts of another. Therefore an image arises of the perceiver (the target individual in this case) carrying out a conscious analysis that leads to a complete and explicitly formulated set of attributions about the other. Several elements of this image are frequently incorrect. First, we are frequently unaware of the attributional processes we employ. Like many other habitual processes, they are both very rapid and automatic, and they need not be very logical or rational in character. Second, they need not be complete; we frequently infer only one or two conclusions about another from his or her actions. At other times our interpretations are minimal, consisting largely of, for instance, an emotional discomfort provoked by the actions of the other. It is important to remember that the interpretations a person makes of another may be incomplete and fragmentary, and some of the future consequences that were discussed in the previous sections may not occur.

Still, as the symbolic interactionists remind us, actions always require interpretations before people can react to them, so some interpretation always occurs. And sometimes the interpretations are sufficiently detailed to generate future consequences. If the target has interpreted the perceiver's behavior as due to the dispositions of the perceiver, then the impression of the perceiver is apt to influence the target's behavior toward the perceiver in later encounters, possibly even in situations very different from that involved in the original interaction, For example, after a brief encounter with the perceiver in a seminar situation, the target may conclude that the perceiver is a warm person. Hence the target may now be more likely to greet the perceiver when encountered in other settings. One of the topics it will be important to address in future research is the conditions that lead to drawing complex attributions versus fragmentary and fleeting nonconscious or emotional reactions.

Four possible categories of attribution may be used by the target to account for the perceiver's actions.

DISPOSITIONAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE PERCEIVER

Most obviously, an attribution may be made directly to the perceiver. For instance, Word et al. (1974) found that subjects rated an interviewer who behaved in a distant manner as less friendly than one who behaved in a more immediate manner. S. C. Jones and Panitch (1971) found that male targets eventually came to like male perceivers who had behaved cooperatively with them and to dislike perceivers who had bargained competitively with them. [3] This experiment is an interesting one in that it illustrates several steps in the general interaction sequence. A perceiver is originally led to believe that a target individual either likes or dislikes him or her. The perceiver then initiates cooperative or competitive behavior. As a result of the perceiver's behavior, the target in fact comes to like or to dislike the perceiver.

Once the target comes to this kind of generalization about the perceiver, that expectation can influence his or her behavior toward the perceiver in a variety of other future situations. Furthermore, the disposition attributed to the perceiver may be generalized so as to be considered characteristic of anyone who falls within the same category membership as the perceiver. For example, a child's first experience with a teacher may lead to generalizations about the ways in which all teachers behave.

ATTRIBUTIONS TO THE SITUATION

The target may make attributions to elements of the situation rather than to dispositions of the other actor. Such an attribution appears particularly likely to occur when the target is attempting to understand the nature of the situational forces. For instance, a subject in the Schachter (1959) fear and affiliation studies, on observing the fear reactions of another subject, may conclude not that the other subject is a fearful person but that the situation in which they both find themselves is generally a fear-provoking one. This kind of situation attribution will affect the target's present and future behavior in that setting, and if the specific setting is perceived as representative of a more general class of settings, then the target person develops expectations about all of those settings. A child whose first classroom is a formal and strictly disciplined one may conclude that this is the behavior required in all classrooms and may have trouble responding in less structured situations. As this example suggests, setting generalizations and role generalizations can be very similar. In one, all settings of a certain kind are seen as requiring certain behavior; in the other, all people of a certain category are expected to behave in a certain way.

THE TARGET'S SELF-ATTRIBUTIONS

The target can be aware that the perceiver's actions toward him or her may be based on some characteristic of himself or herself. The characteristic may be a personal one, or it may be a function of category membership. A person may be aware, for instance, of appearing formal in initial contact situations and may judge that the formal reaction received from the perceiver was elicited by his or her own formality. Or a black person may be aware that some whites may initially exhibit distant behavior because of his or her skin color. Of course, a good deal of research on actor-observer differences suggests that individuals too readily draw dispositional attributions about other individuals. This suggests that individuals are likely to minimize their own role as causal agents in generating the actions of other individuals. However, research (e.g., Arkin & Duval, 1975) has demonstrated that this process is by no means total or irreversible; people are sometimes aware that they cause other people's actions, or can be made to be so aware.

If the target regards something about himself or herself or his or her category membership as a cause of the perceiver's behavior, then this needs to be dealt with in future interactions with the perceiver. On one hand, the target may not accept the validity of that conclusion. Therefore, the target may attempt to counteract and change the perceiver's initial impressions. Or as Mead (1934) and Cooley (1900) have pointed out, the target may come to accept the self mirrored in the perceiver's actions.

COMPLEX ATTRIBUTIONS

More complex attributions about the mening of the perceiver's action are also possible. By and large they involve attributing the cause of the perceiver's action to interactions of the three determinants already discussed. A Person X Situation explanation could occur such that the target decides that the perceiver responds to situations of one type in a particular manner. For instance, the target may conclude that the perceiver reacts to unstructured situations with anxiety. Alternatively, the target may conclude that the perceiver's actions are a result of an interaction between the target and the situation. That is, the target decides that he or she, in the particular type of interaction setting, caused the perceiver to act a certain way. A black personnel manager may be aware that many individuals may react with anxiety when they meet him or her in an employment interview.

All of these interactional attributions for the perceiver's behavior create expectancies about the repetition of that behavior in at least some future settings. Because they are interactional rather than main effect expectancies, they apply to more limited classes of situations. Instead of expecting another individual to react to all situations with anxiety, for instance, we might expect him or her to react with anxiety to all situations in which he or she is confronted with a black authority figure. Insofar as the current scientific conceptualizations of human personality are more consistent with this personality-situation explanation of human action (Mischel, 1968, 1973), it might be argued that these interactionist generalizations are the more sophisticated ones for people to draw. But empirically, this remains to be seen. There is strikingly little research on the target's interpretation of the perceiver's action in an interaction setting.

The Target Responds

Up to this point, we have examined the perceiver's formation of an expectancy, subsequent behavior toward the target person, and the target's interpretation of that behavior. This interpretation is relevant to the manner in which the target will respond.

If the target believes that situational factors or dispositional attributes of the perceiver were responsible for the perceiver's action, the response may be quite different than if the target had decided that it was something about himself or herself that evoked the perceiver's action. The target's response is likely to be coordinated to that of the perceiver if situational forces or the perceiver's dispositions have been identified as causal. Both the original situational forces and the perceiver are still present. In these cases the target is likely to reciprocate the perceiver's action. Approach behavior from the perceiver will lead to reciprocal approach behavior from the target; avoidance behavior will produce avoidance response. If the perceiver has behaved competitively, as in the S.C. Jones and Panitch (1971) and Snyder and Swann (1978) studies involving a game situation, then the target responds competitively. If the perceiver behaves in a friendly, sociable manner, as did the male subjects who were led to believe that they were conversing with a physically attractive female did in a study by Snyder et al. (1977), then the target responds in a sociable manner. It is important to recognize that in each of the above studies, the target's reciprocation of the perceiver's behavior confirmed the perceiver's expectancy. In the first two studies, the target did act, as the perceiver expected, in either a hostile or a nonhostile manner. In the Snyder et al. study, the ostensibly physically attractive female did objectively behave (as scored by judges blind to experimental condition) in a more sociable manner than the ostensibly unattractive female -- just as the male perceiver's stereotype of "what is beautiful is good" led him to expect (cf. Dion, Berscheid, & Walster, 1972).

More subtle nonverbal behaviors are also apt to be reciprocated. For example, Rosenfeld (1967) found that subjects reciprocated with more smiles and positive head nods when treated in that way by the experimenter. Word et al. (1974) also found reciprocation of nonverbal behavior. They trained interviewers to behave in a manner that approximated the behavior exhibited toward the black or white applicants in their earlier experiment. That is, the interviewer sat closer to the applicant, made fewer speech errors, and spent more time with the applicant in an immediate condition than in a nonimmediate condition. Subjects reciprocated Interviewer behavior. Subjects facing the immediate interviewers took more proximate positions and made fewer speech errors than did nonimmediate subjects. Furthermore, raters blind to experimental condition judged the nonimmediate subjects as less calm and composed and less adequate for the job than the immediate subjects.

In most studies of interpersonal expectancy effect, the target's responses to the perceiver bear some reciprocal relationship to the perceiver's actions. On examination, this relationship proves to be of two sorts: first, categorically similar responses, for example, smiles to smiles or more precise speech as a response to a formal interview setting (both of the latter are thought to be indexes of the underlying category of social distance); second, responses that are different in kind from the initial actions that provoked them, for example, flight responses from aggressive actions or distancing responses from what are perceived as sexual advances. As the last example indicates, it is not simply the interpreted action of the perceiver that determines the target's response, but the perceiver's actions as they impact on the target's goals and motives. Approach behavior will lead to withdrawal, not approach, if the target does not want to be approached.

One valid criticism of the self-confirming prophecy literature is that this fact is frequently ignored, leading to an unnecessarily static account of dyadic interaction. In some areas of expectancy-confirmation research, the mediating mechanisms are beginning to be specified and researched. For instance, Rosnow (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1975; Rosnow & Arkin, 1973) and Finkelstein (1976) have pointed out that experimenter expectancy effects require not only the experimenter's signal of expectancies but the subject's attention to and comprehension of those signals. Further, the subject may be motivated to go along with or do the opposite of the experimenter's demands and, finally, may or may not have the capability of doing what he or she is motivated to do. Similar complex mediating models will be needed in other areas of interpersonal interaction research.

Unfortunately, little research has been conducted to examine variables that might determine whether the target will accept or attempt to dispel the perceiver's impression (cf. Gurwitz & Topol, 1978, for one such attempt and Baumeister & Jones, 1978, for another). However, two factors would appear to be critical in determining the nature of that response: (1) the importance of the perceiver to the target and (2) the target's beliefs about the validity of the perceiver's impression. If the perceiver is someone whose opinion is simply not important to the target, then the target is unlikely to be concerned with attempting to dispel the impression. But if the perceiver's impression is important to the target, then the target may attempt to dispel the perceiver's impression if he or she does not believe it to be self-descriptive. Farina, Allen, and Saul (1968) empirically examined this hypothesis and found that subjects who believed that their partner considered them stigmatized in some manner (e.g., mentally ill) worked harder at a cooperative task. Similarly, Lambert, Libman, and Poser (1960) found that both Jewish and Christian subjects increased their pain tolerance after being told that their groups were typically inferior in regard to this attribute.

The target, on the other hand, may find the perceiver's impression to be relatively accurate. In such cases in which the perceiver's impression is congruent with the target's own self-image, the target is likely to behave in a manner that maintains the perceiver's assessment. For example, Baumeister, Cooper, and Skib (1979) found that a female subject who was led to believe that she possessed, and who was believed by a perceiver to possess, a positive (but bogus) trait performed very poorly on a subsequent anagram test if she was led to believe that such poor performance was consistent with this positive trait and if her performance was to be known by the perceiver. Thus given a favorable impression, the target sought to maintain that impression, even if it meant doing poorly on the task.

Further evidence consistent with this notion is provided by some research on the effects of social labeling. In an intriguing field experiment, Kraut (1973) also found evidence demonstrating effects of social labeling. Donors to a first door-to-door campaign were either labeled as charitable by the campaign worker or not labeled at all, whereas non- donors were either labeled as charitable or not labeled. The behavioral evidence on which the label was based apparently led subjects to accept the label as veridical. Subjects labeled charitable gave more, and subjects labeled uncharitable less, to a second charity than did their respective control groups. Thus, a label congruent with the target's own past behavior increased the extent to which later donation behavior was consistent with earlier behavior.

It is also interesting to speculate how the target might respond if uncertain about the fit of the perceiver's impression (cf. Festinger, 1954). Particularly if the perceiver is a significant other who has some expertise and familiarity with making judgments of the sort in question, the target may accept the perceiver's impression and come to believe it. For example, after repeated communications from a professor that he or she is not suited for graduate school, a student may come to believe that he or she in fact is not. If others have reacted to the student in the same manner, then the student is all the more apt to accept that self- definition (cf. Cooley, 1900; Kelley, 1967; Mead, 1934). In such cases, confirmatory behavior from the target may occur (Miller, Brickman, & Bolen, 1975).

The Perceiver Interprets the Target's Response

The target's response is likely to have implications for the perceiver's image of the target. One possibility is that without confirming or disconfirming the original expectancy, the target's action may lead the perceiver to arrive at a set of new expectancies regarding the target's behavior. On the other hand, the target's action may have direct implications specific to that original expectancy. In an objective sense, that is, as interpreted by people who hold no initial expectancy, the target's response may be congruent or incongruent with the perceiver's expectancy and, therefore, may confirm or disconfirm the perceiver's expectancy.

CONFIRMING BEHAVIORS

We have already considered some evidence regarding "objective" confirmation of the perceiver's expectancy. The Word et al. (1974) and Snyder et al. (1977) studies are instances in which naive raters judged target behavior to be characterized by the same qualities that the perceiver had expected. Many of the teacher expectancy studies (e.g., Meichenbaum et al., 1969) demonstrate that differential teacher behavior as a consequence of teacher expectancy produces expectancy confirmation on objective tests of achievement.

How might the perceiver interpret such objective confirmation? Rationally, the perceiver should recognize his or her own role in producing the target's behavior and should attribute the target's behavior to his or her own earlier action. However, a great deal of research suggests that this is not always the case. Observers appear to over-estimate the causal role of dispositional qualities of an actor (E. E. Jones, 1976; E. E. Jones & Nisbett, 1971; Ross, 1977). Even when explicitly made aware of some obvious situational pressures operating on an actor, observers draw dispositional inferences regarding the actor (e.g., E. E. Jones; 1979; E. E. Jones & Harris, 1967; Snyder & Jones, 1974). Basically, perceivers fail to correct adequately for the constraints that a social role or situation may impose on an actor (e.g., Ross, Amabile, & Steinmetz, 1977).

This fundamental attribution error, as Ross (1977) has termed observers' tendency to overestimate the role of dispositional factors and to underestimate the causal significance of situational forces, has implications for the manner in which our perceiver will view the target's response. The perceiver is likely to underestimate the role of his or her own earlier action (e.g., negative nonverbal behavior or friendly conversational demeanor) in determining the target's response. Instead, the perceiver is apt to conclude that the target's behavior was due to, and is an accurate reflection of, the target's disposition. The target is in fact incompetent for the job, friendly and sociable, aloof and hostile, or whatever in the eyes of the perceiver.
admin
Site Admin
 
Posts: 36125
Joined: Thu Aug 01, 2013 5:21 am

Re: Expectancy Confirmation Processes Arising in the Social

Postby admin » Thu Jun 12, 2014 8:01 am

PART 2 OF 2

DISCONFIRMING BEHAVIORS

What if the target's behavior is not in any objective sense congruent with the perceiver's expectancy? Will the perceiver consider his or her original expectancy disconfirmed and consequently modify the assessment of the target person? Research is necessary to identify under what conditions such modification will occur. The available research suggests, however, that such modification on the basis of a single instance of disconfirmation is rare. And it appears that change may be all the rarer, given the behavioral commitment that the perceiver may experience as a consequence of his or her action (Kiesler, 1971). Recent research by Ross, Lepper, and Hubbard (1975) and Ross, Lepper, Strack, and Steinmetz (1977) documents the perseverance of initial impressions. After drawing inferences about some target person, observers learned that the information on which they had based those inferences was bogus. Despite this discrediting of the information, the original impression was found to persevere to a significant degree.

Given that an initial impression perseveres despite the discrediting of the original information, it appears that an initial impression will be all the more likely to persevere in the face of new information that is only inferentially inconsistent with the original information. Thus the perceiver may not abandon the original assessment even if the target's subsequent actions inferentially disconfirm that initial impression. How, then, might the perceiver interpret this disconfirming behavior from the target? A number of studies suggest the existence of an impression-maintenance attributional bias, such that the perceiver attributes expectation- consistent behavior to the dispositional qualities of the target and expectation-inconsistent behavior to situational forces. For example, Regan, Straus, and Fazio (1974) found that a skilled performance (or, in a second experiment, a charitable act) on the part of a liked other was attributed to the dispositional characteristics of that other, whereas the same behavior by a disliked other was attributed to situational forces. Conversely, poor performance was attributed externally for liked others and internally for disliked others. Similar patterns of data have been found regarding the maintenance of expectations concerning physically attractive and unattractive children (Dion, 1972), racial stereotypes (Duncan, 1976), sex role stereotypes (Deaux, 1976), and previous trait ascriptions (Bell, Wicklund, Manko, & Larkin, 1976; Hayden & Mischel, 1976).

Apparently, then, even if a target attempts to dispel a misperception by working hard to perform. skillfully or by complying with a request to perform a charitable act, he or she may not be given full credit by the perceiver for such action. Instead, the perceiver will consider such behavior as unreflective of the target's disposition and as due to situational pressures. The end result, from the point of view of the perceiver, is that the original impression still appears to be accurate.

AMBIGUOUS BEHAVIORS

Of course, many behaviors by the target will not be easily classifiable as confirming or disconfirming of the perceiver's expectancy in any objective sense. Many actions are ambiguous and open to varied interpretation. A great deal of research suggests that ambiguous behaviors tend to be perceived in a biased manner. For example, Hastorf and Cantril (1954) found that Dartmouth and Princeton students viewed infractions during a football game between their two teams very differently. Duncan (1976) found that an ambiguous shove was rated as more violent by white subjects when a black performed the act than when a white did. The interpretation placed on such ambiguous behaviors is usually consistent with the perceiver's initial expectation.

Additional evidence for biased perception processes is provided by an interesting series of experiments by Zadny and Gerard (1974). The findings suggest the operation of an information screening process during the observation of an actor. Intentions earlier ascribed to the actor apparently simplify observation by limiting the actions that are processed. Intent-consistent actions are better recorded and, consequently, better remembered. For example, after being told that an actor was either a psychology, chemistry, or music major, observers witnessed a skit involving the actor. Elements in the skit appropriate to the actor's purported major were better recalled by all three observer groups than inappropriate elements (see also Bruner. 1957). The implication is that informational selectivity will lead our perceiver to process and recall best those elements of the target's ambiguous action that are congruent with his or her expectancy. (See also Snyder & Frankel, 1976.) As a result of such biased perception processes, the perceiver is likely to believe that his or her expectancy has been confirmed.

The Target Perceives His or Her Own Action

The target's response may have objectively confirmed the perceiver's expectancy. The perceiver is likely to have interpreted that action, regardless of its confirmatory or disconfirmatory nature, as consistent (or at least not inconsistent) with his or her impression of the target. But how will the target interpret his or her own action?

Much of the data we have reviewed suggests that the target's response was, in some sense, forced by the perceiver's initial behavior. Furthermore, research on the divergent perceptions of actors and observers suggests that actors are typically more attuned to the situational causes of behavior than observers are. These findings would seem to suggest that the target would attribute his or her response to the situation. However, the actor-observer difference is a relative one; actors tend to attribute more causality to situational factors than do observers. Yet actors, too, tend to underestimate the behavioral limitations that social or situational roles place on them. An interesting experiment by Ross et al. (1977) suggests that actors who know they are assigned on an arbitrary basis to a disadvantageous social role fail to make adequate allowances for that arbitrary role assignment when evaluating themselves relative to the individual arbitrarily assigned to the advantageous role.

Given this underestimation of the power of situational forces, a target person may consider his or her behavior to be relatively self-revealing. A conclusion that the behavior is self-revealing is particularly apt to occur to the extent that the target perceives himself or herself as having freely chosen to perform the behavior. The target is likely to infer attitudes from that action via either a dissonance (Festinger, 1957) or a self-perception process (Bern, 1972). These attitudes inferred by the target might be about (a) his or her beliefs concerning the particular class of situations in which the behavior occurred, (b) his or her beliefs concerning the perceiver or the category of which the perceiver is a member, or (c) his or her beliefs concerning himself or herself.

On the basis of his or her behavior, the target may have learned about his or her attitude toward that particular situation. Such an attitude may guide behavior in similar situations. In the Kraut (1973) study described earlier, donors and non-donors who were respectively labeled as charitable and noncharitable apparently inferred an attitude toward door-to-door charity campaigns. These attitudes influenced their reactions in a later similar situation. Along the same lines, research on compliance via the foot-in-the-door technique suggests that individuals who comply with an initial request self-perceive an attitude toward the situation ("I am the type or person who cooperates with surveyors or campaigners"), which leads them to comply with later requests (Freedman & Fraser, 1966; Seligman, Bush, & Kirsch, 1976; Snyder & Cunningham, 1975). These studies represent instances in which individuals have inferred from their behavior an attitude toward a particular class of situations. In effect, they have learned and internalized a mode of response (or, in Abelson's, 1976, terminology, a script) to a class of situations.

As a second possibility, behavioral inference may lead the target person to form or polarize his or her attitude toward the perceiver (e.g., Davis & Jones, 1960). For instance, after interpreting a perceiver's hostile behavior as due to the dispositional qualities of the perceiver, a target may become all the more convinced that the perceiver is a hostile person on observing his or her own hostile response to the perceiver. ("I wouldn't have responded in that way if he [she] were not such a hostile person.") Such an attitude is apt to influence future behaviors toward the perceiver and may result in continuing confirmation of the perceiver's expectancy of the target and of the target's expectancy of the perceiver.

As a third alternative, the target might infer from his or her behavioral response something about himself or herself. By self-attributing the response, the target may conclude that he or she is the kind of person who typically behaves in that manner or at least is the kind of person who behaves in that manner in situations of that sort. Snyder and Swann (1978) found that targets who were led to believe that their hostile response to a perceiver reflected a disposition acted in a hostile way toward a new partner. This finding represents an extension of the traditional self-fulfilling prophecy -- one in which the target ascribes to himself or herself the very qualities the perceiver expected. This self-ascription, then, prompts the target to behave congruently with the perceiver's original expectancy even in interaction with different others or in different situations. [4]

Clearly the cross-situational generality of such self-ascriptions is in need of further investigation. Nevertheless, some research does suggest that cross-situational generality of a self-ascription can occur. Comer and Laird (1975) found that subjects who agreed to perform an unpleasant task (in this case, eating a worm) tended to draw one of three inferences from their agreement: (1) Some subjects concluded that eating a worm was not actually all that unpleasant; (2) some subjects concluded that they were brave and heroic individuals; and (3) some subjects concluded that they deserved to suffer. The latter two groups of subjects, then, attributed their compliance to some aspect of themselves. These self-ascriptions were found to affect later behavior in a different situation. Subjects were asked to choose. between a task in which shocks were to be administered to the subject and a neutral task. A greater number of those subjects who had modified their conceptions of themselves (Groups 2 and 3 above) chose to shock themselves than did subjects who changed their conception of the worm (Group 1). Thus self-ascriptions inferred from behavior in one situation affected behavior in a new situation. Such general changes in a target's self-concept can be the end result of a perceiver-target interaction that follows the steps we have outlined.

Conclusion

By examining each of the steps in a general interaction sequence, we have noted a number of different forms of expectancy maintenance and confirmation. The classic self-fulfilling prophecy represents only one such form and only one possible outcome of an interaction sequence. Many related outcomes may accrue, however, from the interaction. Let us now review and summarize this variety of expectancy-maintaining and expectancy-confirming phenomena.

1. On the basis of expectancy, the perceiver may avoid or terminate action with the target. Termination of the sequence by a perceiver who holds some power over the target poses a particularly acute problem for the target because of the profound implications it may have on the target's life. Such termination permits the perceiver to maintain his or her impression of the target and effectively prohibits the target from changing that impression..

2. Although the target person's response to the perceiver's action is actually quite ambiguous, the perceiver may interpret the ambiguous response in a biased manner, concluding that the target's behavior actually confirmed the expectancy.

3. Despite the fact that the target's response objectively disconfirms the perceiver's impression of the target, the impression may persevere. The perceiver may attribute the target's disconfirming behavior to situational forces, resulting in expectancy maintenance.

4. The target's response to the perceiver's expectancy-guided behavior may objectively confirm the perceiver's expectancy. That is, naive observers with no expectancy regarding the target would interpret the target's response in a manner congruent with the perceiver's expectations. Such objective confirmation may occur (a) because the target responds in kind to behavior from the perceiver, or (b) because the target accepts as accurate the assessment implied by the perceiver's action and continues to act in terms of that assessment.

5. In the case of objective expectancy confirmation from the target, the perceiver underestimates his or her own role in producing that particular response from the target and attributes the response to the dispositional qualities of the target person. Thus the perceiver believes that the target confirmed the expectancy.

6. The target's self-perception of his or her behavioral response may lead the target to infer a new attitude toward (a) the situation, which may prompt similar confirming behaviors in later similar situations; (b) the perceiver, which may prompt expectancy-confirming behavior in later interactions with the perceiver; or (c) himself or herself, which may represent a modification of self-concept and may influence behavior in a variety of later situations. In each of these cases, and particularly in the last one, the target has in some way been modified as a result of the interaction sequence. The target has discovered something new about himself or herself. He or she has formed new attitudes, polarized former attitudes, or altered some aspect of his or her self-image.

Each of the above interaction effects involves the maintenance and/or confirmation of the perceiver's original impression of the target person. The fourth outcome in the list -- target behavior that objectively confirms the perceiver's expectancy -- is the interaction outcome that fulfills Merton's (1948) definition of a self-fulfilling prophecy. As a result of the perceiver's expectancy and subsequent behavior toward the target, the target person responds in a manner that objectively confirms the expectancy. Generally speaking, such an interaction outcome involves the target person's reciprocation of approach or avoidance behaviors on the part of the perceiver. Given that the perceiver's original expectancy involved some misperception, the interaction has resulted, as Merton noted, in a "reign of error."

The remaining potential outcomes of the interaction sequence either (a) fall short of the traditional meaning of the term self-fulfilling prophecy in that maintenance or confirmation occurs only in the eyes of the perceiver, or (b) go beyond that traditional definition in that the target person has undergone some longer term change. But even those outcomes that fall short of Merton's definition have important practical effects on both the perceiver and the target. The first set of outcomes (Outcomes 1, 2, 3, and 5) involve perceptions on the part of the perceiver that maintain or confirm the assessment of the target person. If the perceiver is in a position of power over the target, then his or her actions can affect the life chances of the target, causing over the long term some very real changes in the target person that are finally consistent with the originally erroneous perceptions of the perceiver. The child assigned to a slow reading group may have less opportunities to learn high-level reading skills (Rist, 1970). Mental hospital patients, the normality of their actions unnoticed or distorted by the custodial staff, may continue to be confined to the mental hospital (Rosenhan, 1973) A black person, denied a job because responses to the unconscious distancing behavior of the interviewer were perceived by that interviewer as signaling hostility, may become hostile after a series of such rebuffs. All of these examples, and many more that could be adduced, indicate the potential social importance of those processes that occur to distort the general social interaction sequence, yet fall short of fitting the definition of the self-fulfilling prophecy.

The sixth form of expectancy confirmation listed earlier represents an extension of the idea of the self-fulfilling prophecy to a process that many have felt is the major source of the social importance of the research. Not only has the behavior of the target been modified by the perceiver's expectancy, but the target's interpretation of that behavior may lead to a change in the self-concept and future behavior of the target. Thus the perceiver's expectancy has exerted an influence that extends far beyond the original interaction and can significantly affect the life of the target person -- perhaps for the better, but as many who do this research fear, often for the worse.

REFERENCES

Abelson, R.F. Script processing in attitude formation and decision-making. In J.S. Carroll & J.N. Payne (Eds.), Cognition and social behavior. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.

Archibald, W.P. Alternative explanations for self-fulfilling prophecy. Psychological Bulletin, 1974, 82, 74-84.

Arkin, R.M. & Duval, S. Focus of attention and causal attributions of actors and observers. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1975, 11, 427-438.

Baumeister, R.F., Cooper, J., & Skib, B.A. Inferior performance as a selective response to expectancy: Taking a dive to make a point. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 424-432.

Baumeister, R.F. & Jones, E.E. When self-presentation is constrained by the target's knowledge: Consistency and compensation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 608-618.

Bell, L.G., Wicklund, R.A., Manko, G., & Larkin, C. When unexpected behavior is attributed to the environment. Journal of Research in Personality, 1976, 10, 316-327.

Bem, D.J. Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6). New York: Academic Press, 1972.

Blumer, H. Symbolic interactionism. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1969.

Brigham, J.C. Ethnic stereotypes. Psychological Bulletin, 1971, 76, 15-38.

Bruner, J.S. On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 123-152.

Chaikin, A.L. & Cooper, J. Evaluation as a function of correspondence and hedonic relevance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1973, 9, 257-264.

Chaikin, A.L., Sigler, F., & Derlega, V.J. Nonverbal mediators of teacher expectancy effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1974, 30, 144-149.

Comer, R. & Laird, J.D. Choosing to suffer as a consequence of expecting to suffer: Why do people do it? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32. 92-101.

Cooley, C.H. Human nature in the social order. New York: Scribner's, 1900.

Cooper, H. Pygmalion grows up: A model for teacher expectation communication and performance influence. Review of Educational Research, 1979, 49, 389-410.

Cooper, J., & Fazio, R.H. The formation and persistence of attitudes that support intergroup conflict. In W.G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The psychology of inter-group relations. Monterey, Calif.: Brooks/Cole, 1979.

Crano, W.D., & Mellon, P.M. Causal influence of teachers' expectations on children's academic performance: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1978, 70, 39-49.

Davis, K.E., & Jones, E.E. Changes in interpersonal perception as a means of reducing cognitive dissonance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1960, 61, 402-410.

Deaux, K. Sex: A perspective on the attribution process. In J.H. Harvey, W.J. Ickes, & R.F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.

DeFleur, M.L., & Westle, F.R. Verbal attitudes and overt act: An experiment on the salience of attitudes. American Sociological Review, 1938, 23, 667-673.

Dion, K.K. Physical attractiveness and evaluation of children's transgressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1972, 24, 207-213.

Dion, K.K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1972, 24, 285-290.

Duncan, B.L. Differential social perception and attribution of intergroup violence: Testing the lower limits of stereotyping of blacks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1976, 34, 590-598.

Dusek, J.B. Do teachers bias children's learning? Review of Educational Research, 1975, 45, 661-684.

Elashoff, J.D., & Snow, R.E. Pygmalion reconsidered. Worthington, Ohio: Charles A. Jones, 1971.

Farina, A., Allen, J.G., & Saul, B.B.B. The role of the stigmatized person in affecting social relationships. Journal of Personality, 1968, 36, 169-182.

Festinger, L. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 1954, 7, 114-140.

Festinger, L. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1957.

Finkelstein, J.C. Experimenter expectancy effects. Journal of Communication, 1976, 26, 31-41.

Freedman, J.L., & Fraser, S.C. Compliance without pressure: The foot-in-the-door technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 195-202.

Gurwitz, S.B., & Topol, B. Determinants of confirming and disconfirming responses to negative social labels. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1978, 14, 31-42.

Hamilton, D.A. Cognition-attributional analysis of stereotyping. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12). New York: Academic Press, 1979.

Hastorf, A.H., & Cantril, H. They saw a game: A case study. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1954, 49, 129-135.

Hayden, T., & Mischel, W. Maintaining trait consistency in the resolution of behavioral inconsistency: The wolf in sheep's clothing? Journal of Personality, 1976, 44, 109-132.

Heider, F. Social perception and phenomenal causality. Psychological Review, 1944, 51, 358-374.

Heider, F. The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley, 1958.

Jensen, A.R. How much can we boost I.Q. and scholastic achievement? Harvard Educational Review, 1969, 30, k1-123.

Jones, E.E. How do people perceive the causes of behavior? American Scientist, 1976, 64, 300-305.

Jones, E.E. The rocky road from acts to dispositions. American Psychologist, 1979, 34, 107-117.

Jones, E.E. & Davis, K.E. From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press, 1965).

Jones, E.E., & DeCharms, R. Changes in social perceptions as a function of the personal relevance of behavior. Sociometry, 1957, 20, 75-85.

Jones, E.E., & Harris, V.A. The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1967, 3, 1-24.

Jones, E.E., & McGillis, D. Correspondent inferences and the attribution cube: A comparative reappraisal. In J. H. Harvey, W.J. Ickes, & R.F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.

Jones, E.E., & Nisbett, R.E. The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Press, 1971.

Jones, R.A. Self-fulfilling prophecies: Social, psychological, and physiological effects of expectancies. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977.

Jones, S.C., & Panitch, D. The self-fulfilling prophecy and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1971, 7, 356-366.

Kelley, H.H. The warm-cold variable in first impressions of persons. Journal of Personality, 1950, 18, 431-430.

Kelley, H.H. Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 15). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1967.

Kiesler, C.A. The psychology of commitment. New York: Academic Press, 1971.

King, A.S. Self-fulfilling prophecies in training the hard core: Supervisors' expectations and the underprivileged workers' performance. Social Scientist Quarterly, 1971, 52-, 369-378.

Kraut, R.E. Effects of social labeling on giving to charity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1973, 9, 551-562.

Lambert, W.E., Libman, E., & Poser, E.G. The effect of increased salience of a membership group on pain tolerance. Journal of Personality, 1960, 28, 350-357.

Lanzetta, J. T., & Hannah, J.E. Reinforcing behavior of "naive" trainers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1969, 11, 245-252.

Lindsay, P.H., & Norman, D.A. Human information processing: An introduction to psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1977.

Mead, G.H. Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1934.

Mehrabian, A. Significance of posture and position in the communication of attitude and status relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 1969, 71, 359-372.

Meichenbaum, D.H., Bowers, K.S., & Ross, R.R. A behavioral analysis of teacher expectancy effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1969, 13, 306-316.

Merton, R.K. Social theory and social structure. New York: Free Press of Glencoe, 1957.

Miller, D.T., Norman, S.A., & Wright, E. Distortion in person perception as a consequence of the need for effective control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 598-607.

Miller, R.L., Brickman, P., & Bolen, D. Attribution versus persuasion as means for modifying behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 430-441.

Mischel, W. Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley, 1968.

Mischel, W. Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review, 1973, 80, 252-283.

Newcomb, T.M. Autistic hostility and social reality. Human Relations, 1947, 1, 69-86.

Palardy, J. What teachers believe -- What children achieve. Elementary School Journal, 1969, 69, 370-374.

Regan, D.T., Straus, E., & Fazio, R. Liking and the attribution process. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1974, 10, 385-397.

Rist, R.C. Student social class and teacher expectations: The self-fulfilling prophecy in ghetto education. Harvard Educational Review, 1970, 40, 411-451.

Rosenfeld, H.M. Nonverbal reciprocation of approval: An experimental analysis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1967, J. 102-111.

Rosenhan, D.L. On being sane in insane places. Science, 1973, 179, 250-258.

Rosenthal, R. On the social psychology of the self-fulfilling prophecy: Further evidence for Pygmalion effects and their mediating mechanisms. New York: MSS Modular Publications, Module 53, 1974.

Rosenthal, R. Experimenter effects in behavioral research. New York: Irvington, 1976.

Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1968.

Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R.L. The volunteer subject. New York: Wiley, 1975.

Rosnow, R.L., & Arkin, L.S. The mediation of artifacts in behavioral research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1973, 9, 181-201.

Ross, L. The intuitive psychologist and his (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10). New York: Academic Press, 1977.

Ross, L.D., Amabile, T.M., & Steinmetz, J.L. Social roles, social control, and biases in social-perception processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, 485-494.

Ross, L., Lepper, M., & Hubbard, M. Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: Biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 32, 880-892.

Ross, L., Lepper, M.R., Strack, F., & Steinmetz, J. Social explanation and social expectation: Effects of real and hypothetical explanations on subjective likelihood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, 817-829.

Schachter, S. The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1959.

Schneider, D.J. Implicit personality theory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 1973, 79, 294-309.

Schneider, D.J., Hastorf, A.H., & Ellsworth, P.C. Person perception. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1979.

Seaver, W.B. Effects of naturally induced teacher expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1973, 28, 333-342.

Seligman, C., Bush, M., & Kirsch, K. Relationship between compliance in the foot-in-the-door paradigm and size of first request. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 517-520.

Snyder, M.L., & Cunningham, M.R. To comply or not to comply: Testing the self-perception explanation of the "foot-in-the-door" phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975, 31, 64-67.

Snyder, M.L., & Frankel, A. Observer bias: A stringent test of behavior engulfing the field. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1976, 34, 857-864.

Snyder, M.L., & Jones, E.E. Attitude attribution when behavior is constrained. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1974, 10, 575-600.

Snyder, M.L., & Swann, W.B. Behavioral confirmation in social interaction: From social perception to social reality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1978, 14, 148-162.

Snyder, M., Tanke, E.D., & Berscheid, E. Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, 656-666.

Sutherland, A., & Goldschmid, M. Negative teacher expectation and I.Q. change in children with superior intellectual potential. Child Development, 1974, 45. l852-856.

Thorndike, R.L. Review of Pygmalion in the classroom. American Educational Research, 1968, 5, 708-711.

Warner, L.G., & DeFleur, M.L. Attitude as an interactional concept: Social constraint and social distance as intervening variables between attitudes and action. American Sociological Review, 1969, 34, 153-169.

Wilkins, W. Self-fulfilling prophecy: Is there a phenomenon to explain? Psychological Bulletin, 1977, 84, 55-56.

Word, C.O., Zanna, M.P., & Cooper, J. The nonverbal mediation of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1974, 10, 109-120.

Zadny, J., & Gerard, H.B. Attributed intentions of informational selectivity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1974, 10, 34-52.

Zanna, M.P., & Pack, S.J. On the self-fulfilling nature of apparent sex differences in behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1975, 11, 583-591.

Zanna, M.P., Sheras, P., Cooper, J., & Shaw, C. Pygmalion and Galatea: The interactive effect of teacher and student expectancies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1975, 11, 279-287.

_______________ The contribution of the two authors was equal; order of authorship is alphabetical. The authors are indebted to Nancy Cantor, Joel Cooper, Edward E. Jones, and Steven J. Sherman for comments on earlier drafts of the article.

Requests for reprints should be sent to John M. Darley, Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton New Jersey 08544.

_______________

Notes:

1. It is not the purpose of the present article to prsent an exhaustive review of all of the studies relevant to the process we describe. Instead we cite various studies that directly bear on the specifics of the suggested processes. The literature on various aspects of the self-fulfilling prophecy is vast. The reader who is concerned with specific areas is referred to various literature reviews such as Dusek (1975), Rosenthal (1976), and Wilkins (1977).

2. Nonverbal behaviors may be of particular importance in social interactions and in the mediation of self-fulfilling prophecies. As Schneider, Hastorf, and Ellsworth (1919) have pointed out, nonverbal behaviors tend to be unintentional in nature. As a result, the perceiver's attitude toward the target person is apt to have a profound influence on nonverbal behavior toward the target (cf. Mebrabian, 1969), and such behavior is apt to be considered reflective of the perceiver's dispositions by the target.

3. Interestingly, the effect did not occur with respect to female dyads, despite evidence that the liking manipulation was effective for females. S.C. Jones and Panitch (1971) reported data showing that female perceivers were less likely than male perceivers to translate their reported liking or disliking into cooperative or competitive behavior toward the target, and also that female targets were less likely than male targets to interpret cooperative or competitive behavior as stemming from the perceiver's likability or dislikability. Although the reason for the sex difference is unclear, the data do demonstrate the necessity of the mediating processes. Only when the manipulation led to differential game behavior did the target, in fact, come to like or dislike the perceiver.

4. It should be noted, however, that the second interaction in the Snyder and Swann (1978) study occurred in the same situation as did the target's original response. Hence we do not know whether the target concluded that he was generally a hostile person or (perhaps more likely) that it was appropriate to behave in a hostile fashion in the confines of competitive laboratory games. To the extent that the latter is true, the Snyder and Swann study may actually represent an example of subjects' inferring an attitude and learning a mode of response toward a particular situation.
admin
Site Admin
 
Posts: 36125
Joined: Thu Aug 01, 2013 5:21 am


Return to Mumia Abu-Jamal

Who is online

Users browsing this forum: No registered users and 2 guests